Which kind of promotion gets you to spend more—a 50% off discount or a “Buy One Get One Free” offer?
The short answer: a Buy One Get One Free offer performs better than its economically equivalent 50% discount.
The longer answer: a Buy One Get One Free offer performs better than its economically equivalent 50% discount, but it depends.
The it depends is what this post will cover. We’ll cover it in four parts:
- First, we’ll take a look at instances where each offer is effective.
- Next, we’ll look at four reasons why a Buy One Get One Free offer tends to be more effective than a 50% discount for the same product, even though both offers are economically equivalent.
- Then, we’ll explore the six exceptions to the rule—when and why people might prefer a discount to its economically equivalent bonus.
- Finally, we’ll look at some practical steps you can take to maximize your offers.
The bad news: it will take a bit to cover all this.
The good news: I’ve summarized all the existing research into this handy flowchart:
Want a higher-res 8½ x 11 PDF of this flowchart? I’d be happy to send one to you:
What do we mean by “discount” and “bonus”?
Before we go too much further, let’s briefly define some terms, just to make sure we’re on the same page.
A discount describes a change in the price. When a product is promoted at 50% off, the product stays the same and the price drops. For example, when a $10.00 bottle of shampoo is promoted at a 50% discount, you walk out of the store with the same bottle, but you’ve paid $5.00 instead of $10.00.
A bonus describes a change in the product. When a product is promoted with a bonus, the price stays the same and the product changes. For example, when a $10.00 bottle of shampoo is promoted with a bonus, you pay the same $10.00, but you walk out of the store with something different. If it’s a buy-one-get-one offer, you walk out with two bottles. Or if the label on the bottle says “50% more shampoo!” then you walk out with a larger bottle.
Bonuses come in a variety of forms, such as:
- A percentage of additional product, such as “50% more shampoo!”
- A second product, such as a “buy one get one” offer.
- The additional product you get when multiple units are purchased, such as a “Buy one, get one 50% off,” “Buy two get the third free,” “Three for the price of two,” and other variations of product quantity and discount.
With that out of the way, let’s dive in.1
Do people prefer discounts or bonuses?
In general, people prefer bonuses. In one study, everyone was shown the base model of a car listed at $10,320. The catch was the some of the people saw a discounted vehicle, while others saw the extra features presented as free extras. People who saw discounts rated the car’s quality a 4.95 on a scale of 1 to 7. But people who saw extra features rated the car’s quality a 5.27. People also valued the car more when extra features were offered: they rated the value a 5.07 on average, compared to a rating of 5.18 from people who saw a discount instead. In short, a bonus made the car slightly more appealing, and made people slightly more likely to buy it.2
Another study tracked a promotion for 9-ounce bottles of hand lotion normally priced at $13.50. During the 16-week study, the promotion alternated each week between a 35% off discount and a 50% bonus. The bonus outperformed the discount by a wide margin, even though a 35% off discount is almost economically equivalent to a 50% bonus:
- In the discount version, the price was 35% off $13.50, or $8.76. For a 9-ounce bottle, this works out to $0.96 per ounce.
- In the promotion version, the price stayed the same, but the bottle sized increased to 13.5 ounces. At a $13.50 price, this works out to $1.00 per ounce.
The bonus promotion also sold more: an average of .56 units per day, compared to an average of .31 units per day for the discount promotion, and it outsold the discount promotion six of the eight two-week periods of comparison. In fact, the store sold 73% more product when they promoted it with a bonus.3
If bonus packs work so well, then why are products put on sale?
Because discounts work, too.
Research on consumer behavior backs this up. A study by William D. Diamond of the University of Massachusetts found that shoppers for laundry detergent liked discounts better. He showed 40 people a 64-ounce bottle normally priced at $4.00. Some saw a discount, while others saw the extra product at the same price. When discounts got bigger, people preferred discounts over bonuses. Diamond also learned that people only like bonuses when they see promotions for extra product, not extra value. For example, “get 16 ounces of extra detergent” performs far better than the economically equivalent “get a dollar’s worth of extra detergent.”4
Another study found that discounts work better than bonuses for museum membership renewals. Some members were offered a 15% discount, while others were offered two extra months. Even though these options are nearly economically equivalent—two free months of the year is worth the same as a 16.7% discount—people preferred discounts to promotions 5.586 to 4.469 on a 7-point scale. (Curiously, this wasn’t true for new members; discounts were less effective at getting new people to sign up.)5
4 reasons bonuses work better than discounts
While both bonuses and discounts are effective, as a general rule, bonuses work a little better than discounts in most cases. Let’s explore the four reasons why this is.
1. Consumers frame losses and gains differently
People make decisions not only based on the content of the decisions but by how those decisions are framed. And a product promotion, by definition, is a different way to frame a decision to buy.
One of the easiest ways to frame a decision is by framing it as a loss or a gain. A discount results in a reduced loss in money. A bonus results in the gain of extra product. For example, a 50% bonus and a 33% discount may be economically equivalent, but one is framed as a loss (or a reduced loss) and the other as a gain.
Why does this matter? Because people are more likely to be affected by losses than gains; the desire to avoid a loss is greater than the desire for its equivalent gain. For example, would you take a bet on a coin toss, where you win $10.00 if it lands heads, but lose $10.00 if it lands tails?
At first glance, this is irrational. A coin toss has a 50% chance of landing heads and a 50% chance of landing tails. This means the expected value is the same:
- Heads: $10.00 × 50% = $5.00
- Tails: $10.00 × 50% = $5.00
As you can see, the expected value of both outcomes is equal, yet most people don’t take the bet because $5.00 lost is worth more than $5.00 gained.
Two psychologists, Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman, found that it takes a win of $22.50 against a loss of $10 for most people to take the bet.
- Heads: $22.50 × 50% = $11.25
- Loss: $10.00 × 50% = $5.00
This shows us that, for this specific bet, even though $10 is worth less than $22.50, a $10 loss hurts as a much as a $22.50 gain feels good. 6
This difference is called the loss aversion ratio. It describes the ratio in how people value losses to gains. For this coin toss bet, the loss aversion ratio is 2.25, although for most decisions the loss aversion ratio is roughly 1.31. In other words, a $10 loss is valued equally to a $13.10 gain.7
As we’ve seen, discounts and bonuses that may be economically equivalent produce divergent consumer responses:
- Discounts can be viewed as reduced losses in money or gains in money.
- Bonuses can be viewed as gains in product or reduced losses in money.
At the same time, a difference in framing can’t fully explain why people generally prefer a bonus to its economically equivalent discount. It can only explain why framing changes behavior. In doing so, it just pushes the question further back:
- Why do consumers sometimes experience discounts as reduced losses in money and at other times gains in money?
- Why do consumers sometimes experience bonuses as gains in product and at other times reduced losses in money?
Next, we’ll dig a little deeper by taking a close look at the perception of numbers themselves.
2. Consumers are bad at calculating percentages
Numbers are tricky things.
For example, 50 is a bigger number than 33. Does that mean 50% is bigger than 33%?
You might be inclined to answer yes, but before you do, consider the following.
It’s true that sometimes 50% is bigger than 33%:
- While stuck in traffic, I have two alternate options for getting home: one that takes 50% longer and one that takes only 33%.
- When I see two promotions: one that offers 50% more shampoo and one that offers 33% more shampoo.
But other times, 50% is not bigger than 33%:
- When I choose between 50% more product and 33% off the price.
- When I see an advertisement telling me Brand A is 50% better than Brand B, instead of an advertisement telling me Brand B is 33% worse than Brand A.
That’s the big difference between regular numbers and percentages: bigger numbers are always bigger than smaller numbers, but bigger percentages are not always bigger than smaller percentages.
The reason this gets you confused is that you sometimes compute differences in percentages as if they were differences in absolute magnitude.8
Here are two examples:
- When two brands are compared, people like Brand A if it is framed as 25% better than Brand B, as opposed to saying Brand B 20% worse than Brand A, even though both are the same.9
- When percentage discounts are stacked, people are more likely to buy. Specifically, they’re more likely to buy something discounted 25% and then marked another 20% off compared to a discount of 40%, even though a stacking discount of 25% and 20% is economically equivalent to a 40% discount.10
3. Consumers neglect the base value
We just saw how people calculate percentages as if they were regular numbers, when, in fact, percentages don’t work the same way.
But how, exactly, are percentages not regular numbers?
Here’s how: they require a base value.
If something is 50% more or 50% less, you need to ask less than what? The “than what” is the base value.
In this way, percentages aren’t absolute, they are always relative to a base value. And if there’s one thing you can count on humans to do, it’s that they’ll neglect the base value.
Researchers tested this in an experiment with 120 consumers. They were shown one of two promotions:
- Regular price of $3.89 for an 8-ounce bottle with a special promoting 50% more free
- Regular price of $3.89 for an 8-ounce bottle, with a special promoting 35% off the regular price.
If you look closely, you’ll see that the two promotions are economically equivalent. But, as expected, people opted for “50% more free” because it sounds like a greater amount than “save 35%”. They didn’t pay attention to the base value of $3.89.
As a follow-up, researchers gave extra emphasis to the $3.89 number, and then re-ran the experiment. When they did, people were more likely to make a quick calculation—which made them just as likely to choose both the discount or the bonus.3
What does this mean? As a default, people are prone to making quick decisions and using mental shortcuts, like “50 is bigger than 35, so 50% must be bigger than 35%.” But when people are prompted to consider their starting point, they’re more likely to do the math—which means they’re equally likely to choose from two economically equivalent options.
4. Consumers have a hard time comparing numbers close together
There’s another wrinkle. People have a hard time comparing numbers that are close together. In fact, it takes a few milliseconds more to compare 25 and 50 than it takes to compare 10 and 11.11
The same is true of percentages: when the numerosity of percentages is close, you’re forced to pause for a few extra milliseconds and consider the numbers you’re looking at. As a result, you’ll make a more conscious and effortful comparison between a discount and a promotion, too.
In other words, you’re more likely to choose a bonus pack over an economically equivalent discount when the bonus pack offer is larger. This is because as the promotion gets better, the numerosity of the percentage between a bonus and a discount gets bigger, too, as you can see in the far-right column in this table:
Product price | Unit size (oz.) | Discount percent | Bonus percent | Difference in percentages |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 5% | 5.26% | 0.26% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 10% | 11.11% | 1.11% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 15% | 17.65% | 2.65% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 20% | 25.00% | 5.00% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 25% | 33.33% | 8.33% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 30% | 42.86% | 12.86% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 35% | 53.85% | 18.85% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 40% | 66.67% | 26.67% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 45% | 81.82% | 36.82% |
$10.00 | 12 oz. | 50% | 100.00% | 50.00% |
To test this, researchers offered bonus and discount promotions at three levels:
- In the first scenario, customers were offered either a 50% bonus or a 33% discount (a difference of 17%).
- In the second scenario, customers were offered either a 100% bonus or a 50% discount (a difference of 50%).
- In the third scenario, customers were offered either an 11% bonus or a 10% discount (a difference of 1%).
They found that people perceived the difference in percentages to be large in the first two scenarios, but not in the third scenario. They also found that people in the 50% bonus/33% discount condition preferred a bonus to a discount more than people in the 100% bonus/50% discount condition and the 11% bonus/10% discount condition. In fact, people tended to prefer a bonus pack at 100%-50% and 11%-10% at roughly the same rate.3
What’s going on here? People prefer the bonus under two conditions: 1) when it’s easy to calculate the difference between the two, and 2) when the numbers are bigger compared to smaller.
The bottom line: customers prefer bonuses to discounts because they’re bad at math, at least most of the time.
6 Exceptions: When consumers prefer discounts to bonuses
There are, however, six specific exceptions to the people-are-bad-at-math rule—instances where the kind of product under consideration overrules the default psychology behind calculation and comparison that would normally lead people to prefer bonuses.
1. Consumers prefer discounts for vice products and prefer bonuses for virtue products
What’s a vice product? You can answer that question for yourself. It’s the kind of product you feel guilty about buying.
Usually, a bonus works better at getting you to buy products. But not vice products. When you think a product is a vice product, the opposite happens: you prefer discounts.
Two researchers confirmed this with a novel experiment. They offered people chocolate priced at $14.00. Half of the subjects saw the chocolate presented with a note about its healthy attributes: the chocolate was sugar-free, made from low-fat milk, and included healthy nuts. But the other half saw the chocolate described as a “sublime consumption experience.”
Each half was split again. Some saw the chocolates promoted with a 20% off discount. The others saw a smaller box of chocolates promoted with a 20% bonus. All participants from both groups saw a box of 42 pieces of chocolate with a final price of $11.20.
What happened? The people who saw the chocolate described using its healthy properties liked the bonus: 62.5%, compared to 37.5% who preferred the discount. But when eating the chocolate was described as a “sublime consumption experience,” their preferences switched: 66.1% of people responded to discounts, while only 33.9% chose the bonus.12
In another experiment, people were shown either raisins or chocolate at either a regular price, a discount, or with a bonus. Then they were asked to indicate how likely they were to buy it. The researchers found that discounted chocolates were 3.125 times more likely to be purchased than chocolates that came with a bonus.12 Tellingly, a bonus did not encourage people to buy chocolate: people who saw bonuses were just as likely to buy chocolate at the regular price without a bonus.
In other words, a “20% more chocolate free!” on your package will not increase the number of units sold. All those people would have bought the chocolate anyway at the regular price.
Why do vice products make you prefer discounts instead of bonuses?
Because you feel guilty about buying vice products. Feelings of guilt have more influence on your desire to buy than your desire for more of whatever you’re buying.
However, let’s face it, you still want to buy vice products. Guilt alone won’t deter you. All you need is a reason to buy That Thing You Know You Shouldn’t Buy.
Both a bonus and a discount can serve as a reason to buy, but they produce opposite effects. A bonus just gives you more of the thing you’re already guilty about getting in the first place. More of That Thing You Know You Shouldn’t Buy isn’t going to tip you over the edge into buying it. That’s why bonuses can work against your desire to buy.
However, a discount helps justify your decision without adding to your guilt.
Here’s how we know this. A discount is just one way to remove guilt from the transaction. But there are other ways to remove guilt; and when guilt is removed in those other ways, people go right back to preferring discounts.
There are lots of ways to remove guilt. Here are two.
2 ways you get rid of guilt for buying a vice product
1. You prefer discounts to bonuses for vice products, unless your friend buys them for you.
Let’s go back to our chocolate example. Sure, you feel guilty when you get it. But what if you don’t buy it? What if someone buys it for you?
In a different version of the chocolate experiment, people were told a friend would pick it up for them. That way they wouldn’t have to buy it themselves. As before, chocolates were described in terms of either their healthy or indulgent properties. When people bought chocolate for themselves, 71.11% chose the discount, 22.22% chose the bonus, and 6.67% were indifferent.
But when people had a friend pick up chocolate for them—when the decision was out of their hands, thereby removing the possibility of feeling guilty—46.34% chose the discount, 48.78% chose the bonus, and 4.88% didn’t care.12
To test this, researchers followed up the experiment, except this time they swapped out the chocolate and offered a salad instead. Now, nobody feels guilty for buying a salad.
When people bought a salad for themselves, 30.3% chose the discount, 66.67% chose the bonus, and 3.03% were indifferent. When people asked a friend to get the salad for them, 41.46% chose the discount, 56.1% chose the bonus, and 2.44% were indifferent.12
What’s the difference between chocolate and a salad? One is good for you, one is bad for you. If a restaurant wants you to buy a salad, they should offer a bonus (or just a really big salad). But to get you to buy dessert, they need to offer you a good price.
2. You prefer discounts to bonuses for vice products, unless you have recently donated to charity.
There’s an even more clever way to feel less guilty: do something nice.
Think about it: what makes you feel guilty?
Usually, you feel guilty after you’ve done something bad.
But if you do something nice, then you’ve lowered your “default guilt” setting. This, in turn, means you need less evidence that you’re a not-guilty person.
You prefer discounts to bonuses for vice products unless you do something nice right before you buy. When you do something nice before you buy, you go back to preferring bonuses to discounts.
Evidence backs this up. In an experiment, 199 people were asked to indicate their willingness to buy a product, on a scale of 1 to 7. Here’s what happened:
People who did not make a donation:
- chocolates: 4.1 price discount, 2.94 bonus pack
- Raisins: 2.65 price discount, 3.98 bonus pack
People who made a donation:
- chocolates: 4.32 price discount, 4.42 bonus pack
- Raisins: 4.06 price discount, 2.58 bonus pack
People were more persuaded by a discount promotion than a bonus promotion unless they recently gave to charity. Those who gave to charity were more likely to be persuaded by a bonus promotion than a discount promotion.12
To sum up what we’ve covered: People need a justification for their purchase. When a bonus pack is offered, the extra quantity is this justification. But for vice products, this extra quantity exacerbates preexisting feelings of guilt. (If you know you shouldn’t eat chocolate, offering more chocolate won’t make you more likely to buy chocolate.) Instead, for vice products, a discount can assuage your guilt, making you more likely to buy. And, surprisingly, other forms of guilt-mitigation, such as giving to charity, influence your preferences, too.
The takeaway: next time you see chocolate, you’re more likely to buy it when there’s a discount unless you threw a few coins in the Salvation Army bucket on your way into the store, in which case a bonus is more likely to get you to buy.
2. Consumers prefer discounts for experiential products if they are introverts or feel socially excluded
Some products are designed to be experienced with other people, such as a trip to Disneyland, dinner at a fancy restaurant, board games, sports equipment, and so on. For these kinds of products a bonus—a “plus one”—works particularly well.
Experiential products—whether they are consumed with others or alone—lend themselves well to bonus promotions. When you go out for dinner, or go on vacation, or buy another kind of similar experience, the happiness you derive from that experience is enhanced in the presence of others. When you see a bonus promotion for an experiential product, like a “two for the price of one” message, it’s usually easy to imagine who you will (or could) experience it with.13
But what if you’re alone? (*sad trombone*) What if you’re an introvert? What if you feel (or are) socially excluded?
If you’re introverted or you feel socially excluded, then you are less likely to respond to a bonus promotion for an experiential product, and more likely to respond to a discount promotion instead.
In a study, when bonuses were described in terms of being used with a friend, people were 56.2% more likely to choose them. The effect was enhanced when people were extroverts—the more extroverted people are, the more likely they are to be persuaded by a bonus promotion for an experiential product. But this effect disappeared when the consumption was framed in terms of self-consumption.14
3. Consumers prefer bonuses for stock-up products and prefer discounts for non-stock-up products
Researchers have also found that discounts have more value for stock-up products than for non-stock-up products.
Before we explore why this is, let’s define what we mean by “stock-up” and “non-stock-up”:
- Stock-up products can be purchased in bulk, are used frequently, and don’t go bad. For example, there’s a 25-pound bag of rice in my pantry, a 10-pack of lightbulbs in my closet, and a 12-count package of Kleenex boxes in my basement. These items don’t go bad and are guaranteed to get used, eventually.
- Non-stock-up products are small, used infrequently, or are perishable. Obvious examples include the milk and produce in my fridge, which must be consumed within a few days. Other examples include non-perishable items where having more than one might not be practical, such as a lawn mower or a hammer. I’ll probably have more than one flat tire in my life, but it doesn’t make sense to carry more than one spare in my trunk, because the storage costs are too high. 15
In a study of 438 customers, researchers asked people to rate the transaction value of product promotions on a scale of 1 to 5. They found that for stock-up products (like bags of rice, light bulbs, and Kleenex), customers preferred discounts and bonuses almost equally: discounts were rated a 3.27 and bonuses were rated a 3.05. However, for non-stock-up products, people strongly preferred discounts to bonuses: discounts were rated 4.07, while bonuses were ranked only 3.05.16
This makes sense if you think about it. If I can consume a gallon of milk in one week, a buy-one-get-one won’t affect my decision to buy, because the extra gallon will spoil before I can drink it. I’ll end up with the same amount of milk at the same price—not exactly a reason to buy.
On the other hand, an economically equivalent 50% off discount makes the transaction more appealing.
Another experiment found that discounts are more appealing for frequently-used products. A 12-pack of lightbulbs is just as nonperishable as the hammer in my garage. But the lightbulbs and the hammer differ in an important way: I’ll use all the lightbulbs within the next couple of years, but I won’t need another hammer for decades. Offering a hammer I’ll never use along with the hammer you want me to buy won’t give me an extra reason to buy; the transaction value is the same.
Every item you own comes with a storage cost. Lightbulbs are small and easy to store. Spare tires, television sets, and cars aren’t. This is why you might occasionally see extreme discounts on large, stock-up products, but you’ll never see a five-for-the-price-of-ten promotion on a new television set.17
4. Consumers prefer discounts at low promotion levels and bonuses at high promotion levels
Researchers have also found bonuses and discounts vary in their appeal depending on the promotion level. In other words: people react differently to a low promotion level—say, 10% off or 10% extra—compared to a higher promotion level, such as 25%, 50%, or 100% off.
But how? In one study, 261 customers were shown tubes of toothpaste at varying promotion levels. (Remember, toothpaste is a stock-up product, and on stock-up products, customers tend to prefer bonuses to discounts.)
Customers saw one of three promotion levels: 10%, 25%, and 50%.
Here’s what the researchers found. For low and medium promotion levels, discounts and promotions are valued almost the same. On the ranking scale used by the researchers, customers who saw a promotion at 10% liked bonuses better: they ranked bonuses at 16.05 but ranked discounts at only 15.34. Customers who saw the 25% promotion ranked discounts and bonuses about the same: they ranked discounts at 18.33 and bonuses at 18.00. But at 50%, customers clearly preferred discounts: they ranked discounts at 23.05 and promotions 20.37.18
Researchers found similar responses for other kinds of products, such as soap, laundry detergent, hand lotion, and trash bags.
What does this mean? All other things equal, you prefer bonuses to discounts at low promotion levels, and you prefer discounts to bonuses at high promotion levels.
5. Consumers prefer discounts for high-priced products and bonuses for low-priced products
Just as the promotion level changes your preferences for discounts and bonuses, so does the price of the product. For lower-priced products, you prefer bonuses, but for higher-priced products, you prefer discounts.
In one study, people were shown a promotion on a product for either “33% more” or “33% off.” If you pay close attention, you’ll notice that 33% off is economically equivalent to 50% more, not 33% more. Yet, as we’ve seen, people view the numerosity of percentages incorrectly: bigger numbers are better. When customers were asked to rank the transaction value on a 7-point scale, they rated a “50% more” promotion a 4.72 but rated a “33% off” a 4.
However, when people were shown a “33% more” promotion, they rated it a 3.76 on average, compared to a 4 for “33% off.” For low priced items, people liked the discount and the bonus roughly the same: they rated the discount at 4.29 and the bonus at 4. But for higher priced items, they preferred the discount, rating discounts a 4, compared to 3.13 for bonuses.3
Again, all things equal, you prefer bonuses to discounts for inexpensive products, but you prefer discounts to bonuses for expensive products.
6. Consumers prefer discounts for unfamiliar products and bonuses for familiar products
You also tend to avoid bonuses if you’re not familiar with a brand.
In an experiment, researchers showed 107 participants various products. Overall, consumers preferred a 50% bonus to a 33% discount, as expected.
But what was striking was that for familiar brands, consumers preferred a 33% bonus to a 33% discount, even though the discount was a better deal. On a 7-point scale, consumers rated the discount a 3.36 but rated a bonus as a 4.3
In another experiment designed to determine the role of risk in consumer preferences for bonuses and discounts, 322 people were shown either a package of AA batteries or a painkiller. Of the people who saw batteries, half saw a normal product, and the other half saw batteries that lasted 15 times longer but were twice as expensive. Of the people who saw painkillers, half saw one that provided relief for four hours, while the other provided relief for 72 hours, but, as with the batteries, was twice as expensive. In short, longer-lasting products came with an increased risk: products that last longer and cost more also are more likely to not perform as expected.
Consumers were asked to rank both the transaction value and their purchase intention on a 7-point scale. The experiment found that for low-risk products, people prefer bonuses to discounts: 4.34 compared to 3.42 for the painkillers, and 4.18 compared to 3.81 for the batteries. But for high-risk products, the opposite happens. People rated a discount at 4.35 compared to a bonus at 3.54 for the painkillers, and they rated a discount at 4.96 compared to a bonus at 4.28. In both cases, their purchase intent decreased as well.19
The reason you prefer discounts when you’re unfamiliar with a brand is that you’re risk-averse. This makes sense: if you’ve never heard of a product, or you haven’t used it, or you don’t know if you trust it, then the prospect of having more of that thing you haven’t heard of, haven’t used, and might not trust won’t convince you to exchange your money for it.
On the other hand, a discount can help you feel better about taking the risk. A discount effectively compensates you for the uncertainty you might feel about a new product or experience. Likewise, a discount can incentivize a brand switch. Remember, discounts don’t reduce the risk, they merely lower the cost you’ll incur if you make the wrong choice. Same risk, just a slightly less bad outcome if you fail.
In short, people don’t want more of something they’re unfamiliar with. Familiar items come with low risk. Unfamiliar items come with higher risk. Because people are risk-averse, a bonus promotion for an unfamiliar brand is ineffective.
Discounts vs. bonuses: some takeaways
Let’s summarize what we’ve covered so far. We’ve seen that, as a general rule, consumers prefer bonuses to discounts because they frame losses and gains differently, they’re bad at calculating percentages, they neglect the base value when comparing discounts and bonuses, and they compare percentages differently depending on how close together the nominal percentages are. We’ve also explored six important exceptions to this rule—instances when customers are more likely to prefer discounts to bonuses: when they purchase vice products, when they feel socially excluded, when they purchase stock-up products, when the promotion level is low, when the price is low, and when the brand is unfamiliar.
Our preferences for different kinds of promotions reveals uncomfortable truths about human nature. We avoid risk. We don’t like taking risks, even if the outcome benefits us. We look for easy ways to feel less guilty. If we do something we regret, we try to justify it. We desire social connections with others, and we spend money on experiences in ways that reflect the presence or absence of bonds with our friends and family.
One thing is clear: knowingly or not, the way in which you respond to various kinds of promotions is largely outside of your conscious awareness.
Want more content like this?
Roughly once per month I’ll send you a new article just like this one. You’ll learn how to make better decisions, understand your customers, and grow your revenue.
Who subscribes? CEOs, professors, marketers, designers, salespeople, non-profit leaders, and investors. Join them by signing up right now.
“I highly recommend subscribing to Kent Hendricks’s newsletter. Imagine if The Atlantic and Wait But Why had a child, who grew up to become an esteemed psychology researcher / game theorist. That’s kind of what reading his work is like.”
—Jeffrey Kranz, Co-founder/CEO, Overthink Group
- What I’m showing you below represents the findings from the best research in the field. But I need to be up front: that research is pretty limited. Here’s how limited: there have been only a couple dozen experiments that compare bonuses and discounts. That’s it. Worse, some of these experiments suffer from small sample sizes, and a few probably won’t replicate. So, while I believe that was I present below is accurate and conclusive, I also feel compelled to tell you that we simply can’t answer some key questions on this topic because the evidence is preliminary or nonexistent.
- Munger, J. L. & Grewal, D. (2001). “The effects of alternative price promotional methods on consumers’ product evaluations and purchase intentions.” Journal of Product and Brand Management, 10 (3), 185-197.
- Chen, H., Marmorstein, H., Tsiros, M. & Rao, A. R. (2012). “When more is less: The impact of base value neglect on consumer preferences for bonus packs over price discounts.” Journal of Marketing, 76 (4), 64-77.
- Diamond, W.D. (1992). “Just what is a ‘dollar’s worth’? Consumer reactions to price discounts vs. extra product promotion.” Journal of Retailing 68 (3), 254–70.
- Byun, J. & Jang, S. (2015). “Effective promotions for membership subscriptions and renewals to tourist attractions: Discount vs. bonus.” Tourism Management 50, 194–203.
- Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1992). “Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5 (4), 297–323.
- Walasek, L., Mullett, T., & Stewart, N. (2018). “A meta-analysis of loss aversion in risky contexts.” SSRN.
- Heath, T., Chatterjee, S. & France, K. (1995). “Mental accounting and changes in price: The frame dependence of reference dependence.” Journal of Consumer Research 22, 90–97.
- Kruger, J. & Vargas, P. (2008). “Consumer confusion of percent differences.” Journal of Consumer Psychology 18 (1), 46–61.
- Chen, H. & Rao, A. (2007). “When two plus two is not equal to four: Errors in processing multiple percentage changes.” Journal of Consumer Research 34 (3), 327–40.
- Thomas, M. & Morwitz, V. (2009). “The ease-of computation effect: The interplay of metacognitive experiences and naive theories in judgments of price difference.” Journal of Marketing Research 46, 81–91.
- Mishra, A. & Mishra, H. (2011). “The influence of price discount versus bonus pack on the preference for virtue and vice foods.” Journal of Marketing Research 48, 196–206.
- Caprariello, P. A., & Reis, H. T. (2013). “To do, to have, or to share? Valuing experiences over material possessions depends on the involvement of others.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104 2), 199–215.
- Lim, S., Cha, O., & Choi, I. (2016). “Buy one get one to share: Preference between bonus packs and price discounts for experiential versus material products.” In NA – Advances in Consumer Research 44, eds. Page Moreau and Stefano Puntoni. Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research, 536–536.
- Litvack, D. S., Calantone, R. J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1985). “An examination of short-term retail grocery price effects.” Journal of Retailing 61, 9–25.
- Sinha, I., & Smith, M. F. (2000). “Consumers’ perceptions of promotional framing price.” Psychology and Marketing 17, 257–275.
- Li, S., Sun, Y., & Wang, Y. (2007). “50% off or buy one get one free? Frame preference as a function of consumable nature in dairy products.” The Journal of Social Psychology 147, 413–21.
- Hardesty, D. M., & Bearden, W. O. (2003). “Consumer evaluations of different promotion types and price presentations: The moderating role of promotional benefit level.” Journal of Retailing 79 (1), 17–25.
- Lowe, B. (2010) “Consumer perceptions of extra free product promotions and discounts: The moderating role of perceived performance risk.” Journal of Product & Brand Management 19 (7), 496–503.